g , between hemispheres (Engel et al , 1991, Engel et al , 2001 a

g., between hemispheres (Engel et al., 1991, Engel et al., 2001 and Buzsáki et al., 2003), between entorhinal cortex and hippocampus (Chrobak and Buzsáki, 1998), and between remote regions of the cerebral cortex (Gregoriou et al., 2009 and Melloni

SAHA HDAC et al., 2007). Candidates for the mediation of these synchronization phenomena are (1) reciprocal fast-conducting glutamatergic projections that originate from pyramidal cells and impinge on both inhibitory and excitatory neurons in the respective target structure and (2) long-range inhibitory projections that directly link the inhibitory network in one region with that in another (Buzsáki et al., 2004, Jinno et al., 2007 and Caputi et al., 2013). In addition to implementing fast-conducting synchronizing connections, nature seems to rely also on counter-intuitive properties of nonlinear dynamical systems that permit such www.selleckchem.com/products/AG-014699.html synchronization by reciprocal coupling despite conduction

delays (Vicente et al., 2008). The most precisely synchronized cortical rhythm is the fast “ripple” oscillation of the hippocampus (130–160 Hz in rats; Buzsáki et al., 1992 and O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978). The frequency of the ripple decreases somewhat from approximately 160–180 Hz in mice (Buzsáki et al., 2003) to 110 Hz in humans (Bragin et al., 1999; Supplementary Note 2); ripples can arise at any site along the septo-temporal axis of the hippocampus and can remain either localized or spread to the septal or temporal direction (Patel et al., 2013). science The ripple-related synchronous hippocampal output can exert a powerful influence on widespread cortical and subcortical structures in both rats and monkeys (Siapas

et al., 2005 and Logothetis et al., 2012), and appropriate timing of these widespread regions demands structural support. It is not known though whether hippocampal ripples activate their different cortical and subcortical targets by delays, in which case their synchrony would not be guaranteed, or whether their target “hot spots” are coactivated to form a specific engram. Under the latter scenario, one might expect special constraints on the transmission pathways and mechanisms, both of which should scale with brain size. In summary, the preservation of temporal constants that govern brain operations across several orders of magnitude of time scales suggests that the brain’s architectural aspects, such as scaling of the ratios of neuron types, modular growth, system size, inter-system connectivity, synaptic path lengths, and axon caliber, are subordinated to a temporal organizational priority. Of these components, the changing features of axons across species are best documented.

On days 5–8, mice were trained to avoid an obstacle by the presen

On days 5–8, mice were trained to avoid an obstacle by the presentation of a tone (90 dB, 15 Hz tone; CS) 285 ms before a rung rose (12 mm; US) in the swing phase of their right paws. Steptime is defined as the time needed to place one of the front paws from one rung to the other; and missteps, as the number of touches on the descended rungs. A decrease in post steptime (steptime directly after the CS) over the sessions, implying that mice learn to adjust their stepping patterns to the obstacle, is taken as a measure of associative motor learning. Patch-clamp experiments were performed as recently published (Schonewille et al., 2010). In short, sagittal slices of the cerebellar vermis (250 μm) from adult

mice were made in ice-cold oxygenated “slicing” solution containing (in mM) 2.5 KCl, 1 CaCl2,

3 MgCl2, 25 NaHCO3, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 240 sucrose, learn more and 25 D-glucose. Slices were kept at room temperature (23°C ± 1°C) in oxygenated ACSF containing (in mM) 124 NaCl, 5 KCl, 1.25 Na2HPO4, 2 MgSO4, 2 CaCl2, 26 NaHCO3, 20 D-glucose, and 100 μM picrotoxin. Cyclosporin A (bath applied, 5 μM in 0.5% EtOH) selleck chemicals was added where indicated. Whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were performed using an EPC-10 amplifier (HEKA, Lambrecht) and patch pipettes filled with (in mM) 120 K-Gluconate, 9 KCl, 10 KOH, 3.48 MgCl2, 4 NaCl, 10 HEPES, 4 Na2ATP, 0.4 Na3GTP, and 17.5 sucrose (at pH 7.25). PF-PC LTD was induced by pairing PF and CF stimulation at 1 Hz for 5 min, and PF-PC LTP was induced by PF stimulation alone at 1 Hz for 5 min. Test responses (two pulses at 50 ms interval) were evoked every 20 s in voltage-clamp mode to prevent spontaneous spiking. In all experiments, cells were switched to current-clamp mode for tetanization. All values are shown as mean ± SEM. All p values were determined for mutants against pooled (values used here) and mutant-specific controls (see Table S2), using two-tailed Student’s t Bay 11-7085 test, one-way ANOVA, or ANOVA for repeated measures with a posthoc Tukey test to determine significance between the groups. p < 0.05 was considered statistically

significant. See Supplemental Experimental Procedures for a full description of experiments. We kindly thank R. Avila Freire, M. Rutteman, D. Smeets, J. van der Burg, E. Haasdijk, and E. Goedknegt for their technical assistance, and we kindly thank the Dutch Organization for Medical Sciences (F.E.H., C.I.D.Z.), Life Sciences (M.S., F.E.H., C.I.D.Z.), Erasmus University Rotterdam Fellowship program (M.S., F.E.H.), Senter (Neuro-Bsik, C.I.D.Z.), Prinses Beatrix Fonds (C.I.D.Z.), the SENSOPAC program, C7 and CEREBNET of the European Community (C.I.D.Z.), the United States Public Health Service MH51106 (D.J.L.) and NS36715 (R.L.H.), and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute (R.L.H.) for their financial support. H.J.B. and C.I.D.Z. were supported by Neurasmus B.V. We kindly thank Toyama (Toyama Chemical Co. Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) for providing the T-588.

The results of Buschman et al (2012) open up a new perspective o

The results of Buschman et al. (2012) open up a new perspective on the mechanisms of rule use and task switching by positing that rules are implemented by dynamic functional coupling in the PFC network. This suggests several extensions to the cognitive control model proposed by Miller and Cohen (2001). Rule application may

be enabled by a change in dynamic coupling across PFC neurons, leading to selection of task-relevant—and suppression of irrelevant—assemblies. Rule maintenance could be mediated by sustained coherence in the task-relevant assembly. Bias signals might primarily modulate the timing of activity, rather than changing average activity levels in their target neurons, and they would selectively enhance synchrony between relevant sensory, memory, and motor populations. Overall, this updated version of the model fits nicely with previously established SAHA HDAC roles of coupled Rucaparib mouse oscillations for communication and selection (Singer, 1999; Fries, 2005; Engel and Fries, 2010; Siegel et al., 2012). This study is one of few to date that relates research on oscillations and neural coherence to that of higher-level cognitive processes. The data may cast new light on how to implement compositionality (i.e., the ability to form more complex expressions from elementary symbols using syntactic rules) (Reverberi et al.,

2012; Maye and Engel,

2012). A question not addressed in the new study is whether rule processing also involves changes in theta-band (4–8 Hz) or gamma-band (>30 Hz) oscillations, which are both known to occur in PFC and are relevant science for communication of PFC with other brain regions (Womelsdorf et al., 2010; Benchenane et al., 2011). In monkeys, theta-band oscillations in the ACC exhibit rule-specific changes (Womelsdorf et al., 2010). Studies in rodents indicate changes in theta-band coherence between hippocampus and PFC during rule acquisition (Benchenane et al., 2011). Future studies need to clarify the potential role of gamma-band activity for rule use, which in paradigms like binocular rivalry or attention tasks are important for selection of task-relevant assemblies (Singer, 1999; Fries, 2005; Siegel et al., 2012). To establish a complete picture of the role of oscillatory rhythms in rule processing, many aspects of the updated model of cognitive control (Miller and Cohen, 2001) still need to be tested. This includes the exact nature of the bias signals arising from PFC during rule application, as well as the presumed large-scale changes in coherence in the pathways enabled by these bias signals. An important question is whether similar rule selectivity of neural coherence can be observed in other relevant brain structures such as the basal ganglia.

, 2012) In a typical working

, 2012). In a typical working Androgen Receptor antagonist memory experiment, subjects are presented with a list of several items. This is followed by a delay period (usually <10 s) during which no information is presented. Subjects are the shown a test item and must make a response based on the properties of the information stored in their working

memory. In humans, MEG studies and intracranial recordings have reported an increase in gamma power during the delay period of working memory tasks. Importantly, this increase varies with the number of items being held in memory (Howard et al., 2003; Roux et al., 2012; van Vugt et al., 2010). An increase in spike-field coherence during the delay period has also been observed (Pesaran et al., 2002). Several studies in humans have reported sustained theta band cortical activity during the delay

period (Gevins et al., 1997; Jensen and Tesche, 2002; Raghavachari et al., 2001; Scheeringa et al., 2009), pointing to a role for theta in working memory. One objection to this conclusion is that single-unit recording of persistent firing during a working memory task did not reveal any obvious theta rhythmicity (Funahashi et al., 1991). However, rhythmicity can be difficult to detect by analysis of spikes alone and is more easily detected by determining whether spikes are phase locked to the oscillations in the local field potential (Wang, 2010). Consistent with this, experiments in monkeys have shown that persistent firing during a working memory task

next is phase locked to low-frequency (theta/delta) Crizotinib ic50 oscillations in the local field potential, both in extrastriate cortex (Lee et al., 2005; Liebe et al., 2012) and in prefrontal cortex (Siegel et al., 2009). Theta-gamma coupling during working memory has been demonstrated in humans both in cortex (Canolty et al., 2006; Lee et al., 2005) and in the hippocampus (Maris et al., 2011). Gamma power in the hippocampus is modulated by the phase of theta oscillations during working memory retention, and the strength of this cross-frequency coupling predicts individual working memory performance (Axmacher et al., 2010). Importantly, the particular cortical regions demonstrating cross-frequency coupling depend on the nature of the information being held in working memory. The spatial distribution of gamma band power in cortex can be used to predict whether working memory is maintaining an indoor or outdoor scene (Fuentemilla et al., 2010), and the gamma activity with this predictive capability is phase locked to the theta activity. In another study, gamma power at certain sites (primarily in occipital cortex) was shown to depend on the particular letter being viewed, and gamma was found to be phase locked to theta (Jacobs and Kahana, 2009).

Blocking glutamate receptors eliminated these events, and bipolar

Blocking glutamate receptors eliminated these events, and bipolar cells provide the only known glutamatergic input to RGCs. Hence, we conclude that inputs from amacrine cells, bipolar cells, Selleckchem Nutlin 3a and to a lesser extent, the intrinsic K+ conductances of RGCs, all combine to shape and amplify the AAQ-mediated RGC light response. Visual acuity is determined by the size of receptive fields of neurons in the visual system. In the healthy retina, the receptive field

of an RGC is defined by the spatial extent of all of the photoreceptors that influence its activity. By definition, the receptive fields of RGCs in rd1 mice are eliminated after the photoreceptors have degenerated. However because AAQ makes presynaptic neurons light-sensitive, it is possible to measure the spatial extent of their light-driven influence on RGC firing. While this is not a conventional measurement of the RGC receptive field, it does indicate the spatial precision of the AAQ-mediated RGC light response. We illuminated AAQ-treated retinas with small spots (60 μm diameter) of 380 nm light centered on one of the 60 electrodes in an MEA (Figure 3A). In the example shown in Figure 3A, upon switching BMS-354825 mouse from 500 to 380 nm light, the average RGC activity increased in the targeted electrode by ∼81% but not in the surrounding electrodes. In each

of a total of eight targeted spots from three different retinas, only neurons near the targeted electrode exhibited Tryptophan synthase a significant increase in firing (median PI = 0.517; Figure 3B). Since RGCs are detected by only one electrode and they are spaced 200 μm apart, this puts an upper limit on the radius of the AAQ-mediated RGC collecting area of 100 μm. Analysis of electrodes outside the illuminated spot showed that

380 nm light significant decreased RGC firing. Decreased firing was detected in electrodes centered at 300, 500, and 700 μm from the mid-point of the targeted electrode (Figure 3C; Table 1). Hence, RGCs in the center of an illuminated spot are stimulated, whereas those in a surrounding annulus (from 200 to 800 μm) are inhibited. Inhibition in the surrounding RGCs implies that a sign-inverting synapse from a laterally-projecting neuron is involved in transmitting information from the center illuminated area to the surround. Amacrine cells are known to form a mutually inhibitory network, making them the likely source of the inhibitory signal. We determined the optimal wavelength for turning off RGC firing when the AAQ photoswitch is driven from the cis to the trans configuration. First, a conditioning 380 nm stimulus was used to turn on firing and then we measured suppression of firing in response to test flashes of different wavelengths. We found that 500 nm light is best at suppressing activity ( Figure 4A), as expected from previous results ( Fortin et al., 2008).

, 1990) The intensity of Aβ plaque deposition was comparable to

, 1990). The intensity of Aβ plaque deposition was comparable to that found in AD cases (Roberts et al., 1990). The presence of Aβ plaques after acute severe brain trauma was verified in many reports, including studies on fresh surgically excised brain tissue samples (Roberts et al., 1994; Ikonomovic et al., 2004). Aggregation of Aβ and plaque formation constitutes central elements of AD. Aβ is generated from amyloid precursor protein (APP) by the concerted action of β-secretase and γ-secretase (Blennow et al., 2006). β-Secretase was identified as β-site

APP-cleaving enzyme 1 (BACE1), while γ-secretase consists of a complex with four components that include presenilin, nicastrin, Pen-2, and Aph-1. Presenilin is present in the active site of the γ-secretase complex (Blennow et al., 2006). selleck Expression of APP is highest in neurons and, under normal conditions, APP (Koo et al., 1990; Ferreira et al., 1993; Kamal et al., 2000), β-secretase (BACE1) and γ-secretase (presenilin) (Sheng et al., 2003) are translocated by axonal transport to the synapses, where APP can be cleaved by the secretases, thus generating Aβ (Kamal et al., 2001). Extensive research contends that APP has neurotrophic functions, including promotion of axonal sprouting, neurite outgrowth, and synaptogenesis, www.selleckchem.com/products/Adriamycin.html which are important for neuronal survival after axonal damage (Small,

1998; Small et al., 1999; Alvarez et al., 1992; Xie et al., 2003). second APP is upregulated in response to brain trauma and administration of soluble α-secretase-cleaved APP improves functional outcome and reduces neuronal cell loss and axonal injury after experimental TBI in animals (Thornton et al., 2006). Studies on human brain tissue samples have demonstrated that APP accumulates in neurons and axons after brain trauma with axonal damage (McKenzie et al., 1994; Sherriff et al., 1994; Gentleman et al., 1995; Ahlgren et al., 1996; Gleckman et al., 1999). Postmortem studies on human brain tissue samples from patients who sustained mild TBI but died of other causes have shown that

this APP accumulation occurs very rapidly (within a few hours) after brain trauma and is present in cases with mild trauma (Blumbergs et al., 1994; McKenzie et al., 1996; Johnson et al., 2012). Besides APP, acute intra-axonal Aβ accumulation is a prevalent trait in human TBI (Smith et al., 2003; Uryu et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2009). Release of β-amyloid (especially Aβ42) into tissue and plaque formation around damaged axons occurs after APP accumulation and β-amyloid production in damaged axons (Roberts et al., 1991; Graham et al., 1995; Horsburgh et al., 2000a; Smith et al., 2003). Studies on brain trauma induced by rotational acceleration in animal experiments show an accumulation of APP and Aβ within damaged axons throughout the white matter, which in a subset of animals is accompanied by Aβ diffuse plaques (Smith et al.


“The segregation of continuously varying stimuli into disc


“The segregation of continuously varying stimuli into discrete, behaviorally relevant groups, a process referred to as categorization, is central to perception, stimulus identification, and decision making (Freedman and Assad, 2006, Freedman et al., 2001, Leopold and Logothetis, 1999 and Niessing and Friedrich, 2010). In some cases, the boundary between categories is fixed (Prather et al., 2009). In most cases, however, the boundary needs to adjust according to context, a process referred to as flexible categorization. Recent research suggests that such flexible categorization also contributes to competitive stimulus selection for gaze

and attention (Mysore and Knudsen, 2011b). A midbrain network that plays an essential role in gaze and PI3K inhibitor attention (Cavanaugh and Wurtz, 2004,

Lovejoy and Krauzlis, 2010, McPeek and Keller, 2004 and Müller et al., 2005) OSI-744 nmr segregates stimuli into “strongest” and “others” (Mysore and Knudsen, 2011a). The midbrain network includes the optic tectum (called the superior colliculus in mammals) and several nuclei in the midbrain tegmentum, referred to as the isthmic nuclei (Knudsen, 2011). Categorization by this network tracks the location of the strongest stimulus in real time as a precursor to the selection of the next target for gaze and attention. Despite the importance of flexible categorization to a broad range of functions, how the brain implements it is not known. Categorization by the midbrain network arises from special response properties of a subset

of neurons located in the intermediate and deep layers of the owl optic tectum (OTid) (Mysore et al., 2011 and Mysore and Knudsen, 2011a). These neurons display “switch-like” responses, firing at a high rate when the stimulus inside not their classical receptive field (RF) is the strongest (highest intensity or speed) but switching abruptly to a lower firing rate when a distant, competing stimulus becomes the strongest. This switch-like property causes the encoding of categories by the OTid to be explicit: the category can be read out directly from the population activity pattern without any further transformations beyond simple linear operations, such as averaging (Gollisch and Meister, 2010). In addition, if the strength of the stimulus inside the RF is increased, a switch-like neuron requires a correspondingly stronger competing stimulus to suppress its responses. This property causes the category boundary to be flexible, enabling network responses to reliably identify the strongest stimulus at each moment in time. Explicit and flexible categorization by this network dramatically improves the discriminability of the strongest stimulus among multiple competing stimuli of similar strength (Mysore et al.

, 2012)

, 2012). learn more Envelope ICMs have been addressed in numerous fMRI studies, often using graph-theoretical approaches (Lynall et al., 2010 and Alexander-Bloch et al., 2010). These studies suggest that there is a reduction in functional connectivity that particularly concerns interactions between frontal and posterior regions (Fornito et al., 2012). Graph-theoretical analyses reveal decreased local clustering and decreased modularity, indicating less effective local communication (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2010 and Fornito et al., 2012). However, there are also indications of reorganization

at a global level toward higher efficiency (decreased path length) and increased robustness (resistance to fragmentation after removal of nodes) (Alexander-Bloch et al., 2010). Phase coupling has often been studied in task-related activity patterns (Uhlhaas and Singer, 2012 and Gandal et al., 2012) but less extensively in ongoing activity. Available studies on phase ICMs seem to support the hypothesis of regionally decreased functional connectivity in the alpha (Hinkley et al., 2011) and gamma band (Kikuchi et al., 2011). Overall, a complex pattern of developmentally reorganized coupling is Selleck LY294002 present where connectivity

is not generally reduced but may also involve abnormal increases and, in this sense, schizophrenia may represent a dysconnection, rather than a disconnection, syndrome (Uhlhaas, 2013). Taken together, the studies reviewed above suggest that alterations in envelope or phase ICMs correlate with behavioral or cognitive alterations in the respective disorder. The changes in ICMs seem to differ considerably across disorders, suggesting progressive disconnection in AD and MS, dysconnectivity in schizophrenia, Fossariinae the predominance of an abnormal phase ICM in PD, and altered functional balance across different subnetworks

in stroke. The studies clearly demonstrate that investigation of ICMs can add significantly to our understanding of specific network pathologies and that they can broaden our view on the physiological relevance of network stability, for example, by assessing parameters like robustness as available from graph theoretical analyses (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009 and Bullmore and Sporns, 2012). In several disorders, clear and testable hypotheses on causal relations between changes in ICMs and clinical phenotype have been formulated. A highly relevant insight is that changes in functional connectivity observed in several of these disorders cannot be predicted in a straightforward manner from structural alterations. While numerous studies have addressed BOLD envelope ICMs in neuropsychiatric disorders, almost no neurophysiological studies on envelope ICMs and relatively few studies on phase ICMs are available.

First identified as a factor that controls pigmentation in fish,

First identified as a factor that controls pigmentation in fish, melanocortin receptors also serve to control hair color in mammals. In the brain, the best-studied function is regulation of food intake and metabolism. The results from Lim et al. (2012) also implicate melanocortins in a tightly regulated stress response where they adversely affect reward and hedonic state that is relevant to depression. We know that depression presents itself in many ways, with patients suffering from different symptoms. The parsing of anhedonia and helplessness is therefore critical, and the present work gives specific mechanisms and potentially distinct targets for future therapies. “
“The success of biological systems depends upon their capacity

to adapt to the environment. Over half a century ago, Conrad Waddington proposed that organismal development and reaction to the environment are governed by an “epigenetic system” that sculpts R428 cell line the pathway of embryogenesis

(Waddington, 1942, 1959). Waddington’s elegant metaphor of the “epigenetic landscape” illustrated the alternative pathways that a cell might traverse depending on extrinsic influences and adaptive responses, the topology of this landscape Depsipeptide being defined by a web of underlying gene networks (Waddington, 1957). Although modern usage of the term epigenetics invokes a rather specific set of chromosomal mechanisms that regulate gene expression, Waddington pondered the Isotretinoin relationships between genotype and phenotype before the molecular machinery

could be defined. In fact, Waddington described a genetically encoded adaptive mechanism as “a gun which is not only set on a hair trigger but which is aimed to hit the target when it goes off” (Waddington, 1959), anticipating the structure of cellular signaling to regulate downstream target genes (Figure 1A). We now appreciate that cells possess an extensive arsenal of adaptive signaling mechanisms suitable for responses to a wide range of temporal domains and environmental conditions or cellular interactions (Figure 1B). While rapid and local state changes are effectively triggered by conformational, catalytic, and posttranslational modification of molecules already available in the cell, sustained adaptive state changes can persist beyond the lifetime of individual molecules, such as the memories stored in neural networks. Mechanisms that link adaptive responses to expression of the genome not only provide the renewable resource of RNA and protein, but also can alter the “program” of the cell via qualitative changes in expression (reviewed by Flavell and Greenberg, 2008). Although transcriptional mechanisms can produce very long-lived state change, they offer limited spatial acuity and thus depend on posttranscriptional processes for regulated delivery of the expressed genome. Spatial constraint is particularly important in the nervous system, where extremely complex cell architecture is essential for circuit structure and function.

Data from the current study suggesting an association between fun

Data from the current study suggesting an association between functional gains and physical activity for participants taking more than 398 steps per day could contribute to development of such guidelines. No matter whether current physical activity guidelines for older adults are appropriate for orthopaedic rehabilitation inpatients, the results of the current study suggest that these patients could benefit from being more active. A change to the rehabilitation

ward environment has been shown to reduce the amount of time patients spent at their bedsides but did not increase physical activity levels (Libraries Newall et al 1997) highlighting the need for supervision, encouragement, and a change in attitude of hospital staff who are riskaverse and prefer patients not to mobilise independently. Inpatients in rehabilitation do more physical activity when therapy AZD9291 is being provided (Bear-Lehman et al 2001, Smith et al 2008) and spend little time in self-directed physical activity (Newall et al 1997, Patterson et al 2005, Tinson 1989). This suggests that one potential way of increasing physical activity levels would be to provide additional allied health therapy. INCB018424 datasheet In a recent randomised controlled trial, participants who received physiotherapy and occupational therapy interventions

six days per week had significantly higher physical activity levels than those who received the intervention on five days (Peiris et al 2012a). Results from a qualitative study these of patients in the same setting indicate that patients are agreeable to the additional therapy (Peiris et al 2012b) and the resulting higher levels of physical activity. Other options include group therapy and utilisation of allied health assistants to increase physical activity levels. However, as resources can be limited, efforts need to be made by physiotherapists to implement strategies to empower ward staff, patients, and their carers to increase

physical activity levels outside of therapy. One limitation of our study is that the activity monitor used did not record activity in lying or sitting. However, it has been advocated that doing non-stepping activity such as bed exercises should not be considered mobilisation or a substitute for upright physical activity (Bernhardt et al 2007) and that, in this population, walking is the most important activity to measure (Tudor-Locke et al 2011). In conclusion, patients with lower limb orthopaedic conditions in inpatient rehabilitation are relatively inactive and do not meet current physical activity guidelines. Given the importance of physical activity for general health and functional improvements following hospitalisation it is important to develop methods to decrease sedentary behaviour and increase physical activity levels in rehabilitation. Footnotes: aActivPAL, PAL Technologies, Glasgow.